- Research
- Open access
- Published:
Sex differences in electrical activity of the brain during sleep: a systematic review of electroencephalographic findings across the human lifespan
BioMedical Engineering OnLine volume 24, Article number: 33 (2025)
Abstract
Background
With the explosion of techniques for recording electrical brain activity, our recognition of neurodiversity has expanded significantly. Yet, uncertainty exists regarding sex differences in electrical activity during sleep and whether these differences, if any, are associated with social parameters. We synthesised existing evidence applying the PROGRESS-Plus framework, which captures social parameters that may influence brain activity and function.
Methods
We searched five databases from inception to December 2024, and included English language peer-reviewed research examining sex differences in electrical activity during sleep in healthy participants. We performed risk of bias assessment following recommended criteria for observational studies. We reported results on sex differences by wave frequency (delta, theta, alpha, sigma, beta, and gamma) and waveforms (spindle and sawtooth), positioning results across age-related developmental stages. We created visualizations of results linking study quality and consideration of PROGRESS-Plus parameters, which facilitated certainty assessment.
Results
Of the 2,783 unique citations identified, 28 studies with a total of 3,374 participants (47% male, age range 4–5 months to 101 years) were included in data synthesis. Evidence of high certainty reported no sex differences in alpha and delta relative power among participants in middle-to-late adulthood. Findings of moderate certainty suggest no sex differences in alpha power; and theta, sigma and beta relative power; and delta density. There is evidence of moderate certainty suggesting that female participants had a steeper delta wave slope and male participants had greater normalized delta power. Evidence that female participants have higher spindle power density is of low certainty. All other findings were regarded as very low in certainty. The PROGRESS-Plus parameters were rarely integrated into the methodology of studies included in this review.
Conclusion
Evidence on the topic of sex differences in sleep wave parameters is variable. It is possible that the reported results reflect unmeasured social parameters, instead of biological sex. Future research on sex differences in sleep should be discussed in relevance to functional or clinical outcomes. Development of uniform testing procedures across research settings is timely. PROSPERO: CRD42022327644. Funding: Canada Research Chairs (Neurological Disorders and Brain Health, CRC-2021-00074); UK Pilot Award for Global Brain Health Leaders (GBHI ALZ UK-23-971123).
Introduction
Sex and gender consideration in neuroscience and health research are widely endorsed by major funding organizations, as there is strong evidence to demonstrate that biological and social factors contribute to differences in health outcomes and are, therefore, relevant for precision medicine and person-centered care [1, 2]. Sex refers to biological characteristics, including differences in chromosomes, reproductive organs, and gonadal hormones. Gender refers to sociocultural characteristics, reflecting the roles, responsibilities, identity, and behaviors of girls, boys, men, women and people of other genders [1, 2]. Both sex and gender are interconnected and influenced by environment and culture.
Several studies reported on sex differences in the structure and function of the brain, during both sleep and wakefulness [4, 5]. Such findings urged scientific journals and regulatory agencies to require researchers to consider biological sex in their research and raised awareness that ignoring sex has ramifications both in terms of rigor and reproducibility of research, potentially leading to costly consequences and unrealized benefit [4, 5]. However, sex interacts with several environmental, developmental, and genetic factors and should, therefore, be investigated in the context of other parameters that influence brain structure and function [6].
Evidence emerged suggesting that social experiences alter neuronal structure and function, and can shape epigenetic influences on central nervous system (CNS) development and decline [7, 8]. This discussion raises an important question regarding the degree to which the observed group-level differences are attributable to biological sex or socio-cultural gender [3], highlighting the need to examine how genetics, environment, and behavior influence neuroplasticity processes across the human lifespan.
Electroencephalography (EEG) is a non-invasive neuroimaging technique that allows researchers to observe dynamic processes in the brain as they happen. It is used to capture electrical activity of the brain during both sleep and wakefulness and allows for the analysis of frequency waves, absolute and relative spectral powers, asymmetry of spectral powers between the right and left hemisphere, intra- and inter-hemispheric magnitude, coherence and phase synchrony, and many other parameters [9]. This technique can be applied to a wide range of scientific inquiries, from the study of cognitive processes in psychology to the study of neural engineering and rehabilitation [10, 11].
Several studies have reported that sex differences exist in EEG frequency waves, in both healthy people and people with a wide range of conditions. For example, a recent study of sex differences in resting EEG in healthy young adults found that females had greater overall amplitudes in delta, alpha, and beta activity during the resting state, enhanced midline activity in theta, and parietal and midline activity in the alpha and beta bands, concluding that these findings indicate significant differences in neuronal activity between young adult female and male persons [12]. In the sleeping state, sex differences were observed in EEG functional connectivity in healthy people [13]. Synchronization intensities showed differences in all sleep stages: higher in female in non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep, and higher in male persons in alpha and beta bands in rapid eye movement (REM) sleep [13].
Recent research leveraging deep learning to predict a person’s sex using clinical EEG data of over 1000 people aged 18 to 88 reported that sex was detectable by neural networks with 81% accuracy [14]. Researchers found that the predictive ability was primary driven by EEG topographies, rather than waveforms and frequencies, emphasizing the need for future research considering age, health status, and state in which EEG data is collected (e.g., sleep versus wakefulness) to minimize confounding effects.
In this review, we aimed to synthesize evidence on sex differences in sleep wave parameters in healthy persons, utilizing the PROGRESS-Plus framework (i.e., Place of residence, Race/ethnicity/culture/language, Occupation, Gender/sex, Religion, Education, Socioeconomic status, Social capital, and other characteristics, including age) [15, 16]. We hypothesized that if we observe consistently reported differences in sleep wave parameters between male and female persons during ages that are linked to sex steroid hormone differentiation (i.e., adolescence, menopause) but not during adulthood stages (regardless of the extent of the PROGRESS-Plus consideration), then the difference is more likely driven by biology (i.e., sex), as opposed to social or environmental factors. However, if this hypothesis is not supported by the evidence, then social and environmental factors (i.e., PROGRESS-Plus parameters) are more likely driving the observed differences. We therefore conducted a systematic review to: (1) identify and critically appraise original studies that investigated sex differences in sleep wave parameters in healthy participants using EEG; (2) analyze data by sleep wave parameter and age-related developmental stages; and (3) interpret evidence through the PROGRESS-Plus lens [16].
Results
Study selection
We identified 3,648 records within the databases searched on November 22, 2021, and 757 records within the repeated databases searched on December 13, 2024. One additional record was located manually [17]. After the removal of duplicates, we screened 2,784 records from the initial searches and 564 records from the repeated searches. We identified 63 citations for full text review, 28 of which met inclusion criteria for data synthesis. Reasons for exclusion for the remaining 35 studies were recorded (Supplementary Material S1) and displayed in the PRISMA flow diagram (Fig. 1). The characteristics of the 28 included studies are reported in Tables 1, 2, and Supplementary Material S2.
Study sample characteristics
The 28 studies included in the systematic review involved a total of 3,374 participants (47% male participants) [5, 17,18,19,20,21,22,23,24,25,26,27,28,29,30,31,32,33,34,35,36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43]. The percentage of male participants in each of the included studies ranged from 33% [34] to 67% [38]. The age of participants (in years) ranged from 4-5 months [42] to 101 [36, 37]. The smallest sample size was 16 participants [33] and the largest was 822 participants [31].
Twenty-eight studies reported on sex differences among their participants [5, 17,18,19,20,21,22,23,24,25,26,27,28,29,30,31,32,33,34,35,36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43]. One study (4%) included participants solely in the infancy and toddlerhood stage of development (4–5 months of age) [42], six studies (21%) included participants soleley in early, middle, and late childhood and adolescence [5, 20, 22,23,24,25]; five studies (18%) included participants solely in emerging and early adulthood [18, 19, 26, 30, 32]; one study (4%) examined middle adulthood [35]. No studies investigated sex differences solely in the late adulthood developmental stage. Fifteen studies (54%) included participants spanning age-related developmental stages [17, 21, 27,28,29, 31, 33, 34, 36,37,38,39,40,41, 43]. Detailed characteristics of the included studies can be found in Table 1.
EEG markers and parameters
Overall characteristics
Twenty-three studies (82%) reported results on sex differences in the parameters of the delta wave frequency [5, 17,18,19,20,21,22,23,24, 26,27,28,29,30,31,32,33,34,35,36,37, 39, 43]. Sex differences in the theta wave frequency were reported in 14 (50%) studies [5, 17, 18, 20, 21, 23, 24, 26, 27, 30,31,32, 36, 43], alpha in 14 (50%) studies [5, 17, 18, 20, 21, 24, 27, 30,31,32, 35, 36, 41, 43] and sigma in 12 studies (43%) [5, 17, 20, 21, 24, 26, 30,31,32, 40, 41, 43]. Ten studies (36%) reported on sex differences in beta wave frequency [5, 17, 20, 21, 24, 31, 36, 40, 41, 43] and three studies (11%) in gamma [5, 24, 41]. Six studies (21%) reported on spindles [5, 25, 27, 36, 38, 42] and one study (4%) on sawtooth [36] waveforms.
We observed considerable between-study variations in the recording techniques, in the frequency band definitions and categorization among the included studies, irrespective of whether the data emerged from EEG as part of polysomnography, or standalone EEG. We refer the reader to Table 2 for definitions of the studied parameters.
We observed variations in the reporting of the age of participants. Twenty-two studies (79%) reported age of their samples as means and standard deviations (SD) [5, 17,18,19,20, 22, 24, 26, 28, 29, 31,32,33,34,35,36, 38,39,40,41,42,43]. Fifteen studies (54%) reported both mean with SD and age ranges [5, 17, 26, 29, 31, 33, 34, 36, 38,39,40,41,42,43], and three studies (11%) [25, 27, 37] reported only age range, which in some cases spanned several decades (Table 1, Fig. 2). This variation required us to use age-related developmental stages to contextualize the evidence and preserve all the data in data synthesis (Fig. 2). This allowed us to make comparisons of results across similar age groups. The decision to combine specific age-related developmental stages was made since few studies had a sample that could be categorized under a singular age-related development stage. Studies reported on several sleep wave parameters (Fig. 3).
Sex differences in sleep parameters, by brain wave frequencies. Color indicates sex differences in sleep marker values: females greater (pink), males greater (blue), no difference (yellow), grey (not reported). Superscript indicates parameter(s) investigated in the study: A, amplitude; C1, coherence; C2, mutual information; C3, weighted-phase lag index; D’, density; D1, power density; D2, change in power density; D3, density peak; P’, power; P1 relative power; P2, change in power; P3, normalized activity. Length of bars indicates the age range of participants in each study; a circle indicates that a cohort of a single age was included. Please refer to the Results section and Fig. 3 for studies reporting multiple sex difference results
Sex differences in sleep parameters, by brain wave frequencies. Labels indicates the parameter investigated in the studies: A, amplitude; C1, coherence; C2, mutual information; C3, weighted-phase lag index; D’, density; D1, power density; D2, change in power density; D3, density peak; P’, power; P1 relative power; P2, change in power; P3, normalized activity. Color indicates sex differences in sleep marker values: females greater (pink), males greater (blue), no difference (yellow). Length of bars corresponds to number of PROGRESS-Plus parameters considered in statistical analysis of the study, shown as a superscript: G, gender/sex; + , plus (additional parameters, shown in parentheses). Line style corresponds to Quality Assessment of the study: Excellent (+ + , thick solid lines), Good (+ , medium dashed lines), Fair (-, thin dotted lines)
Sex differences in delta waves
Twenty-three included studies (82%) provided relevant information on sex differences in delta frequency waves [5, 17,18,19,20,21,22,23,24, 26,27,28,29,30,31,32,33,34,35,36,37, 39, 43], across various age-related stages of development: five (18%) of these studies included participants in childhood and adolescence [9, 20, 22,23,24], five (18%) in emerging and early adulthood [18, 19, 26, 30, 32], and one (4%) in middle and late adulthood [35]. Twelve studies (43%) included participants spanning across several age-related developmental stages [17, 21, 27,28,29, 31, 33, 34, 36, 37, 39, 43].
Early, middle, and late childhood and adolescence:
Campbell et al. (2005) reported no sex differences in delta amplitude and power density in the 9-year-old cohort; however, sex differences were observed in the 12-year-old cohort, where male participants expressed higher values in both amplitude and power density compared to female participants [24]. In female participants only, of both age cohorts, researchers observed weak negative correlations between power density and Tanner stage, which was observed in the first recording but not second, which took place 6 months after the first[24]. Campbell et al. [23] reported on change in power as participants aged (i.e., from age 9–16 (cohort 1) and age 12–18 (cohort 2)) [23]. Researchers found that the age of steepest delta power decline differs between the sexes, with decline occurring at an earlier age in females [23]. Feinberg et al. [22] reported no sex differences in power density for the 9-year-old cohort; in the 12-year-old cohort, male participants had greater power density as compared to female [22]. Delta power density did not change between ages 9 and 11 for both sexes; however, it declined between ages 12 and 14 in both sexes, with no sex differences in the rate of decline [22]. Baker et al. [20] found no sex differences in delta power and amplitude in their participants aged 11–14 years [20]. Markovic et al. [5] in their participants aged 9 to 14 years, reported no sex differences in absolute power across brain regions in NREM and REM sleep; however, when the data were normalised, sex differences were observed, with male participants showing greater power in the central/occipital region during both REM and NREM sleep, and female in the frontal region during NREM [5]. This group also reported differences in the connectivity, expressed as coherence, with female participants having greater values in NREM and REM sleep [5]. Ringli et al. [39] observed increased delta power from the ages of 10–17 in male participants in the right frontal region; female participants had higher power values in the left and right temporal regions [39].
Emerging and early adulthood:
Dijk et al. [30] reported that female participants aged 19–27 years exhibited higher delta power density during REM and NREM sleep as compared to their male counterparts [30]. In the research by Mongrain et al. [26] with participants aged 19–34 years, researchers found female participants to have higher spectral power as compared to male participants [26]. Armitage [18] found higher overall delta power during NREM stages 2–4 in their female participants aged ~ 20–30 years; during REM and all-night sleep (REM + NREM), no difference was observed [18]. In a study published five years later with participants aged 22–40, Armitage found no sex differences in the amplitude and power of delta sleep [19]. In a study of delta sleep during napping state in participants aged 18–23, Dorokhov et al. found that female participants had greater spectral power density as compared to male [32].
Middle and late adulthood:
Latta et al. (2005) reported greater absolute delta power in female participants compared to male during both NREM and REM sleep, but no sex difference in relative delta power. When the data were normalised, sex difference for relative delta power was observed, with male participants expressing greater power.
Studies spanning several developmental stages:
Mourtazaev et al. [37] reported greater delta power in female participants in a sample with ages ranging from 26 to 101; the age and sex interaction was not statistically significant [37]. Carrier et al. [27] reported greater power spectral density among female participants aged 20–60, as compared to male [27]; the results were similar in the study by Ma et al. [31], with a sample aged 18–64 [31]. Carrier et al. [28] reported greater delta wave amplitude in female participants. In their analysis of sex differences in density, the researchers found that main sex effect was not statistically significant, but observed statistically significant results for age group, sex, and derivation interaction [28]. Fukuda et al. [33] reported greater spectral power in female as compared to male, in a sample of participants aged 54–72 [33]. Hejazi et al. [34] found no sex differences in NREM (N1-N3) delta power in their participants aged 20–56 years old [34]. Kluge et al. [17] found sex difference in delta power in their sample aged 60–70 years old, with females exhibiting higher values [17]. Yoon et al. [43] reported greater absolute delta power in female participants compared to male, but no sex difference for relative delta power in their sample aged 45–69 [43].
Luo et al. [36] reported no sex difference in functional connectivity, quantified using mutual information, in delta wave frequency in both NREM (N1-N3) and REM in their participants aged 25–101 [36]. Rosinvil et al. [29] reported that female showed greater slow wave density, amplitude and slope as compared to male participants in a sample aged 20–71; however, after application of age-and sex-adapted criteria, sex difference in slow wave density became not statistically significant, while the sex differences in the amplitude and slope remained significant [29].
Sex differences in theta waves
Fourteen studies provided results on sex differences in theta waves across developmental stages [5, 17, 18, 20, 21, 23, 24, 26, 27, 30,31,32, 36, 43]. Four of these studies included participants in childhood and adolescence [5, 20, 23, 24] and four in emerging and early adulthood [18, 26, 30, 32]. Six studies included participants spanning developmental stages [17, 21, 27, 31, 36, 43].
Early, middle, and late childhood and adolescence:
In their cohort aged 12 years, Campbell et al. [24] reported no sex differences in theta power density in NREM sleep in the first recording session, however male participants showed greater values in the second recording session [24]. In the study by Campbell et al. [23], researchers investigated change in power, reporting that the steepest decline in theta power occurred at an earlier age in female participants as compared to male [23]. Markovic et al. [5], in their sample of 9–14-year-olds, reported on three parameters: coherence, absolute power, and normalised power. Compared to male, female participants had greater coherence in NREM and REM sleep, with no sex difference observed in absolute power. When power data were normalized, sex differences were observed in certain topographic regions [5]. Baker et al. [20] reported no sex differences in their 11–14-year-old participants in theta power during NREM and REM sleep [20].
Emerging and early adulthood:
Dijk et al. [30] reported that female participants in their sample aged 19–27 years exhibited greater theta power density during REM and NREM sleep as compared to male [30]. Dorokhov et al. [32] reported sex difference in theta spectral power densities, with female participants showing higher power densities across all sleep stages during a nap [32]. In a sample of participants aged 19–34 years, Mongrain et al. [26] found higher power in NREM in female as compared to male participants [26]. Armitage [18] reported no sex difference in all-night sleep theta power in their participants aged ~ 20 to 30 years [18].
Studies spanning several developmental stages:
Ma et al. [31] reported female participants had higher power spectral density as compared to male, in a sample aged 18–64 years [31]. Carrier et al. [27] reported higher power spectral density for female participants as compared to male in a sample aged 20–60 [27]. Kluge et al. [17] reported no sex difference in theta power in their sample aged 60–70[17]. Yoon et al. [43] found female participants had greater absolute spectral power using ANOVA; however, sex was not significant in multivariate modeling accounting for sex and age [43]. The researchers reported greater relative spectral power in female participants during REM sleep, and no sex differences in NREM and whole-night sleep [43]. Yuksel et al. [21] reported no sex difference in relative power in NREM in both recording nights [21].
Luo et al. [36] in their sample of participants aged 25–101 years observed no sex difference in theta wave functional connectivity, quantified using mutual information [36].
Sex differences in alpha waves
Fourteen studies investigated sex differences in parameters of alpha wave frequency in participants across age-related developmental stages: three of these studies included participants in childhood and adolescence [5, 20, 24], three in emerging and early adulthood [18, 30, 32], and one in middle adulthood [35] (Fig. 2). The remaining seven studies [17, 21, 27, 31, 36, 40, 43] included participants spanning across age-related developmental stages.
Early, middle, and late childhood and adolescence:
Campbell et al. [24] reported higher power density for male participants than female in their cohort aged 12 in both recording sessions, which were conducted 6 months apart [24]. Baker et al. [20] found no significant sex differences in alpha power in NREM and REM in their sample of adolescents aged 11–14 years [20]. Markovic et al. [5] found no significant differences in absolute alpha power in NREM and REM in their sample of 9–14-year-olds, although Markovic et al. [5] noted sex differences in normalised power in NREM sleep, with female participants showing greater power in the frontal region and male participants in the central/occipital regions [5]. These researchers also reported on coherence, with male participants showing greater values in NREM, and no sex difference in REM sleep [5].
Emerging and early adulthood:
Dijk et al. [30] found that alpha power densities during NREM and REM sleep were higher in female participants, except for the 9- and 10-Hz during REM sleep [30], in a sample aged 19–27. Armitage [18] reported no significant difference in alpha power between the sexes for ~ 20 to 30-year-old participants [18]. Dorokhov et al. [32] observed that female participants demonstrated higher spectral power densities, compared to male in their sample aged 18–23 [32].
Middle and late adulthood:
Latta et al. [35] found no sex differences in absolute, relative, and normalised alpha wave activity in their sample aged ~ 57–65 [35].
Studies spanning several developmental stages:
Yoon et al. (2021), in their participants aged 45–69 years, reported higher absolute alpha power in female as compared to male using ANOVA; sex was not significant in the multivariate regression accounting for sex and age. No sex difference was observed in relative alpha power during whole night, NREM, and REM sleep [43]. Yuksel et al. [21] found no sex difference in relative power [21]. Kluge et al. [17] found no sex difference in alpha power in participants aged 60–70 years [17]. Ma et al. [31], in their sample aged 18–64, observed a higher power spectral density in their female participants as compared to male, in the frequency band 8–10 Hz and no sex differences for >10–12 Hz [31].
Ujma et al. [41], in their sample aged 17–69 years, reported that males expressed greater alpha wave connectivity, quantified as weighted phase-lag index, in NREM than female participants [40]. Luo et al. [36] observed greater functional connectivity, quantified using mutual information, in the alpha frequency band in female compared to male participants, across all stages of sleep, in participants aged 25–101 years [36].
Sex differences in sigma waves
Twelve studies reported results on sigma wave frequency [5, 17, 20, 21, 24, 26, 30,31,32, 40, 41, 43]. Three studies included participants in childhood and adolescence [5, 20, 24] and three in emerging and early adulthood [26, 30, 32]. The remaining six studies included participants across age-related developmental stages [17, 21, 31, 40, 41, 43].
Early, middle, and late childhood and adolescence:
In their cohort of 12-year-olds, Campbell et al. [24] reported higher power density values in male participants relative to female in the second recording, which occurred six months after the first, during which no sex differences were observed [24]. Baker et al. [20] found no sex differences in sigma power in their sample of 11–14 year-olds [20]. Markovic et al. [5] observed greater absolute sigma power in females during NREM sleep in their cohort of participants aged 9–14 years; and no sex difference in REM sleep [5]. When the data were normalised, researchers observed sex differences during NREM and REM sleep across brain regions [5]. The researchers also reported on coherence, with female participants expressing greater values in both NREM and REM [5].
Emerging and early adulthood:
Dijk et al. [30] found that sigma power densities during NREM and REM sleep were higher in females, except for the 12- and 13-Hz during NREM sleep [30], in their sample aged 19–27. In the study by Mongrain et al. [26] including participants aged 19–34 years, female participants had greater sigma power in NREM [26]. Dorokhov et al. [32] reported that female participants exhibited higher sigma spectral power density as compared to males [32].
Studies spanning several developmental stages:
Ujma et al. [41] in their sample of 17–69 year-olds reported greater connectivity in female participants in the frequency range 13.5–14.5 Hz, and greater values in male participants for the low sigma range [41]. In the study of Ujma et al. [40], researchers reported sex differences in sigma amplitude, in their sample aged 17–69 years [40]. Ma et al. [31] studied power spectral density, reporting no sex difference in the 12 to < 14 Hz range, and greater values among females in the 14–15 Hz range [31]. Kluge et al. [17] reported no sex difference in sigma power in their sample aged 60–70 years [17].
Yoon et al. [43], in their participants aged 45–69 years, reported higher absolute sigma power in female as compared to male participants using ANOVA; sex was not significant in the multivariate regression accounting for sex and age [43]. No sex difference was observed in relative sigma power during whole night, NREM, and REM sleep [43]. Yuksel et al. [21] reported no sex difference in relative sigma power in NREM sleep [21].
Sex differences in beta waves
Ten studies explored sex differences in EEG parameters in the beta frequency range in samples that include participants from various age-related developmental stages: three of these studies included participants in childhood and adolescence [5, 20, 24], and the remaining seven studies included participants across developmental stages [17, 21, 31, 36, 40, 41, 43].
Early, middle, and late childhood and adolescence:
Campbell et al. [24] reported higher density values in the first night recording among males in the 12-year-old cohort; however, no sex differences were observed in the second recording 6 months later [24]. Markovic et al. [5], in their sample of 9–14-year-olds, found that female participants exhibited greater absolute power; when the data were normalised, sex differences were observed in certain brain regions [5]. The researchers also reported on coherence, with female participants showing greater values in NREM and REM [5]. Baker et al. [20] found no significant difference in beta power in their cohort of 11–14-year-old participants [20].
Studies spanning several developmental stages:
Ujma et al. [41] in their sample aged 17–69 observed higher connectivity, quantified as weighted phase-lag index, in male in NREM and REM, as compared to female participants [41]. Luo et al. [36] reported higher functional connectivity, quantified as mutual information, in female compared to male participants in a sample ranging from ages 25 to 101 [36]. In the study of Ujma et al. [40], researchers reported that sex differences exist in beta amplitude, in their sample aged 17–69 years [40]. Ma et al. [31], in the sample of 18–64 year-olds, reported that female participants had a greater power spectral density in the 15–18 Hz range, but no sex difference in beta frequency ranges > 18 Hz [31].
Kluge et al. [17], in their sample aged 60–70, found that male participants had greater beta power [17]. Yoon et al. [43] reported that male participants had lower absolute spectral power in ANOVA, but that sex was not significant in multivariate analysis accounting for age [43]. Males had greater relative power during whole night sleep, but no sex differences were observed in NREM or REM sleep [43]. Yuksel et al. [21] reported no sex differences in relative power [21].
Sex differences in gamma waves
Sex difference in gamma waves in sleep was explored in three studies [5, 24, 41]: two studies included participants in childhood and adolescence [5, 24], and one study included participants spanning across developmental stages [41].
Early, middle, and late childhood and adolescence:
Markovic et al. [5] reported that female participants expressed greater absolute gamma power in comparison to male in NREM and REM sleep, in their sample aged 9–14 [5]. When the data were normalised, sex differences were observed in specific brain regions [5]. The researchers also reported on sex differences in coherence; results indicated that female participants showed greater values in NREM and REM as compared to male [5].
Studies spanning several developmental stages:
Ujma et al. [41] found that male participants exhibited greater connectivity for gamma waves in NREM and REM in their sample of participants aged 17–69 [41].
Sex differences in transient waveform events
In addition to reporting on sex difference in EEG waves in specific frequency bands, six groups of researchers reported on sex difference in transient waveforms events known as sleep spindles [5, 25, 27, 36, 38, 42]. One group also reported on sawtooth waves [36], a variant of theta activity, with each wave also containing a notch, making it sawtooth-shaped.
Sex difference in spindles were reported in participants in infancy and toddlerhood in one study [42], in childhood and adolescence in two studies [5, 25], and in participants spanning across developmental stages in three studies [27, 36, 38]. Sex difference in sawtooth waveform was reported in participants spanning age-related developmental stages in one study [36].
Infancy and toddlerhood:
Ventura et al. [42] reported that female participants in their sample aged 4–5 months expressed greater spindle spectral power as compared to male [42]. No sex differences were observed in spindle density [42].
Early, middle, and late childhood and adolescence:
Markovic et al. [5] reported that female participants in their cohort aged 9–14 expressed greater spindle amplitude in comparison to male [5]. The researchers also found greater spindle density in female participants; however, when the analysis was performed in female participants with no menarche and age-matched male participants, females still expressed greater density in slow spindles; but no sex difference was observed in fast spindles [5]. Zhang et al. [25] reported on sex difference in spindle amplitude in their cohorts aged 9 and 12; results were reported by brain region [25]. For both cohorts, male participants had greater amplitude in the central lead as compared to female; no sex difference was observed in the frontal lead [25]. These researchers also reported on the density peak, with results showing no sex difference for central or frontal leads [25].
Studies spanning several developmental stages:
Carrier et al. [27] reported that female participants in their sample aged 20–60 had greater power spectral density and compared to male [27]. Luo et al. [36] reported on sex differences in spindle and sawtooth connectivity, quantified by mutual information, in their sample aged 25–101. Female participants expressed greater spindle connectivity as compared to male, and no sex difference in sawtooth waves [36]. Pun et al. [38] reported no sex difference in spindle density in their participants aged 51–80 [38].
PROGRESS-Plus considerations
A detailed description of the characteristics of included studies using the PROGRESS-Plus framework can be found in Supplementary Material S2, Figs. 4 and 5.
Number of studies reporting at least one PROGRESS-Plus parameter: P, place of residence (blue, n = 6); R, race/ethnicity/culture/language (green, n = 6); O, occupation (yellow, n = 2); G, gender/sex (purple, n = 28); E, education (orange, n = 2); plus, additional parameters (red, n = 28). Stacked bars represent proportion of participants (%) in each category: place of residence, race/ethnicity, language, work status*, sex, and education**. Stacked bars for plus parameters represent number of studies reporting age only (n = 21) or both age and pubertal development (n = 7). *Carrier et al. [27] reported that more than 95% of participants in their sample were students, workers, or homemakers, but did not provide specific breakdown of work status. **Pun et al. [38] reported education in years (mean and standard deviation)
Place of residence of participants
Seven studies reported on participants’ place of residence [20,21,22,23, 35, 37, 43]. These seven studies included participants from Abbotsford (Australia) [20], Davis [21,22,23] and Chicago (USA) [35], Ansan (Korea) [43], and Leiden (Netherlands) [37].
Although only indirectly linked to participants’ place of residence, the country in which research was conducted is important to consider, as participant recruitment often occurs locally. Out of the 28 studies, most studies were conducted in high-income countries. Eleven studies were conducted in the USA [18, 19, 21,22,23,24,25, 27, 31, 34, 35], four in Canada [26, 28, 29, 38], and two in Switzerland [5, 39], the Netherlands [30, 37] and Germany and Hungary [40, 41], each. One study was conducted in each of the following countries: Ireland [42], Australia [20], Germany [17], Korea [43], Japan [33], and Russia [32]. Luo et al. [36] used Sleep-EDF Database from the Research Resource for Complex Physiologic Signals established under the auspices of the National Institutes of Health, but did not specify the country of data origin [36].
Race/Ethnicity/Culture/Language
Five studies reported on race/ethnicity of research participants or their parents [21,22,23, 42, 43] and one study reported on language [26]. Yoon et al. [43] noted in the discussion that race has influence on morphological features due to genetic factors, which may be the reason for the absence of sigma peaks in Koreans [43].
Occupation
Two studies [26, 27] reported on occupation of their participants. The majority of participants (> 95%) in the study by Carrier et al. [27] were students, workers, and homemakers [27]. Out of the 24 participants in the study by Mongrain et al. [26], 18 were students (without a summer job), three worked at home with their own schedule, two were in between jobs, and one participant worked afternoon shifts [26].
Education
Two studies reported on the education level of their participants [32, 38]. In the study by Dorokhov et al. [32], participants were university students [32]. In the study by Pun et al. [38], the researchers reported years of education of their participants: 18.1 ± 4.3 years for males and 17.7 ± 2.7 years for females [38].
Remaining parameters (gender, religion, social capital, and socioeconomic status)
Gender as a sociocultural construct [1], religion, social capital, and socioeconomic status of research participants were not reported in the studies included in this review.
Plus
We observed a variety of statistical approaches and Plus variables considered in the study of sex differences in sleep wave parameters (Supplementary Material S3).
Age
All 28 studies reported the participants’ age as mean and standard deviation, range, or both [5, 17,18,19,20,21,22,23,24,25,26,27,28,29,30,31,32,33,34,35,36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43]. One study reported on gestational age and post-natal age of their research participants [42]. Of these, 18 studies considered age in the analyses of sex difference [5, 17, 19, 20, 22,23,24,25, 27,28,29, 34, 36, 37, 39,40,41,42].
Pubertal stage
Seven studies reported on pubertal stages of their research participants [5, 20,21,22,23,24, 39], of which six used Tanner stage to measure pubertal development [5, 20, 22,23,24, 39] and one used the Pubertal Developmental Scale [21]. Of these, three studies considered Tanner stage in the analyses of sex differences [5, 22, 23].
Risk of bias in studies and certainty of the evidence
We used the NIH tool to rate studies an “excellent” (i.e., low risk of bias), “good” (i.e., moderate risk of bias), “fair” (i.e., high risk of bias) rating (Supplementary Material S4). The criteria for the six potential sources of bias were rated as “Yes”, “No”, “Cannot determine”, “Not applicable” or “Not reported”. If an item was rated as “No”, it was regarded as a potential risk of bias. The challenge faced by study authors in assessing risk of bias using the NIH tool was in designating “Cannot determine” or “Not reported” versus a definite rating. For instance, if little information was provided for Item 5 (“Was a sample size justification, power description, or variance and effect estimates provided?”), the authors were left with no clear indicators to distinguish “Cannot determine” (described all or some components but not in detail) versus “Not reported” (did not describe). Thus, we rated studies that provided at least some information as “Yes” for a conservative evaluation. Likewise, Item 11 (“Were the outcome measures [dependent variables] clearly defined, valid, reliable, and implemented consistently across all study participants?”) was rated “Yes” because the minimal requirement was agreed to be a standardized measure of outcome. Thus, we rated three studies as “excellent” [20, 35, 43], 21 studies as “good” [5, 17, 19, 21,22,23,24,25,26,27,28,29,30, 32, 34, 36, 37, 39,40,41,42], and four studies as “fair” [18, 31, 33, 38] (Supplementary Material S4).
Figure 3 positions results based on frequency bands, sleep wave parameter, quality assessment, and PROGRESS-Plus variables considered in the analysis of sex difference. Using the criteria for certainty assessment outlined in the methods section, sex differences in alpha and delta relative power [35, 43], reported in two studies of excellent quality, were assessed as high in certainty. Results suggest moderate certainty that no sex differences exist in alpha power [5, 17, 20, 43], in theta [21, 43], sigma [21, 43], and beta relative power [21, 43], and in delta density [28, 29]. There is also moderate certainty that female participants had greater delta slope [28, 29], and male participants had greater normalized delta power [5, 35]. Low certainty evidence points to sex difference in spindle power density, with greater values in female [27, 31] compared to male participants. Evidence for all other sleep parameters was assessed as very low in certainty.
Discussion
Summary
In our systematic review, we included 28 studies that assessed sex differences in sleep wave parameters across ages and age-related developmental stages. We did not observe consistently reported differences in sleep wave parameters between male and female persons during ages that are known to be linked to sex steroid hormone differentiation (i.e., adolescence, menopause) and, therefore, we cannot conclude that the reported differences are driven by biology (i.e., sex). The results, nonetheless, provide an opportunity for a unique discussion with both research and clinical implications.
The strongest emerging pattern was that the majority of studies investigating parameters of delta, theta, and spindle waveform reported greater values among female participants as compared to male, or no sex difference (Fig. 2). The majority of these studies included samples that spanned adulthood developmental stages. When it comes to sigma, beta, and gamma waves, there were less consistent results on sex differences. Despite observed trends, the certainty of evidence for most studied parameters was very low. The varied results regarding sex differences in sleep parameters may suggest that physiological and pathophysiological processes in sleep reflect the great variability in a person’s environmental and social influences (i.e., PROGRESS-Plus), as opposed to biological sex, which were rarely considered in data analysis of sex difference. In light of this, we conclude that future studies on sex differences in sleep parameters that integrate social variables hold great potential to advance our understanding of the brain activity in sleep on a lifespan continuum.
Sleep wave parameters
Various sleep waves have been reported to oscillate differently during stages of sleep and levels of wakefulness [44]. The infra-slow oscillations (ISO, < 0.5 Hz), most prominent during non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep, have been suggested to express the overall neuronal connectivity of the brain. During wakefulness, these ISO are less prominent than in sleep, and they have been linked to baseline cortical excitability and large-scale network coordination, influencing vigilance and the transition between sleep and wakefulness [44, 45]. In our review, we did not observe studies that reported on sex differences in ISO, which position these waves as a priority to fill this knowledge gap. This is especially relevant because no studies included in this review connected results on sex differences in sleep wave parameters to EEG activity during waking state.
Delta waves (i.e., usually ~ 0.5 to 4 Hz frequency band, but defined differently by each study author, see Table 2) dominate during deep sleep (N3 sleep, also known as slow wave sleep (SWS)) and are essential for restorative and neuroplasticity processes, including tissue repair and memory consolidation [46]. In the waking state, occurrence of delta waves suggests compromised brain processes, including exhaustion and extreme fatigue [44]. Several studies included in this review reported on sex differences in a number of delta wave parameters, illustrated in Figs. 2 and 3. The studies to date did not, however, link the results concerning sex difference to the restorative and neuroplasticity processes in their study samples.
Theta waves (i.e., usually ~ 4 to 7 Hz frequency band, but defined differently by each study author, see Table 2) emerge during the transition from wakefulness to sleep, and are present during REM sleep [44, 47]. In the waking state, theta activity is associated with drowsiness or a meditative state [46]; however, when excessive, can be indicative of attentional deficits, frustration, annoyance, and embarrassment [48, 49]. Frontal theta waves in the waking state are linked to tasks involving working memory, attention, and emotional processing [50]. In sleep, one study included in this review reported on sex differences in central and occipital region during the childhood and adolescence, with male participants showing greater values [5]. The significance of this finding remains to be established.
Alpha waves (i.e., usually ~ 8 to 12 Hz, but defined differently by each study author, see Table 2), prominent during relaxed wakefulness, particularly when the eyes are closed and the person is in a calm, resting state, have been associated with introspection, relaxation, and inhibition of active processing during tasks requiring focused attention [44]. These waves start to diminish with sleep onset, giving way to slower EEG rhythms [47]. The sex-specific results of studies included in this review were mixed, with some results pointing towards greater values for female participants in alpha power density [27, 30,31,32], and others reporting no differences, primarily in power [5, 17, 18, 20, 43] and relative power [21, 35, 43]. Several studies included participants spanning a wide range of ages, which could affect precision in analysis and reporting, as dominant influences from specific ages in relatively small sample sizes of studies included in the review may skew results for the entire range in either direction [27, 31, 36, 41, 43]. Future studies on sex differences in alpha activity in sleep are greatly needed.
Sigma waves (i.e., usually ~ 12 to 16 Hz frequency band, but defined differently by each study author, see Table 2) play a critical role in memory consolidation and synaptic plasticity [44, 51, 52]. While sigma activity is not typically observed during wakefulness, it represents a key marker of sleep depth and stability [44, 51]. Sex differences in sigma wave parameters reported in studies included in this review were mixed, with no clear direction to discuss the results and their relevance to the waking state (Figs. 2 and 3).
Beta waves (i.e., usually ~ 13 to 30 Hz frequency band, but defined differently by each study author, see Table 2), most prominent during wakefulness, particularly during active cognitive processing, problem-solving, and decision-making, are generally distributed over the frontal and central regions of the brain [44]. During sleep, beta activity decreases significantly but can reappear transiently during periods of arousal or light sleep [44] and in REM [53]. Further exploration on sex differences in beta activity in sleep is needed, since the results of several studies reported contrasting results regarding sex difference in the same parameters, such as power density [24, 30, 43] and power [5, 17, 20, 43] (Fig. 3).
High-frequency oscillations (HFOs, > 30 Hz) include gamma waves (30–80 Hz), ripples (80–200 Hz), and fast ripples (200–500 Hz) [44]. Gamma waves are reported to be associated with higher cognitive functions, including attention, memory encoding, and sensory perception, and are present during both wakefulness and REM sleep [44, 47]. Ripples and fast ripples are typically associated with epileptic activity but, during wakefulness, they may also reflect high-level cognitive processes and rapid information processing [44]. Three studies included in this review reported on sex differences in gamma waves, but on different parameters [5, 24, 41]. The evidence is insufficient to discuss with any degree of certainty and emphasizes the need for further investigations.
Sleep spindles are believed to protect sleep by inhibiting external sensory input, and their frequency and distribution are linked to cognitive performance and development [47, 51]. Sleep spindles are generated by an interplay between neurons in the thalamic reticular nucleus and the relay nuclei and conveyed to the cortex by the pattern of burst firing [54]. Reported sex differences in amplitude, density, power, and connectivity of sleep spindles [5, 25, 27, 31, 36, 38, 42] could be linked to differences in the structure of the thalamus or deafferentation of the cortex, synaptic connectivity among the cell types and corticothalamic projections, differences in neural processing, input from arousal systems and hormones, among many other influences, that are believed to play an important role in maintaining NREM sleep continuity. However, results on sex difference in fast and slow spindles [5] may suggest differences in activation patterns between the two, as fast spindles have been shown to be associated with increased activity in sensorimotor areas, the hippocampus, and the mesial frontal cortex, while slow spindles with activation in the superior frontal gyrus [55]. Study of sex difference in sleep spindles are of interest to different disciplines in health research, due to their implications in learning and memory [56], and in sensory gating [57, 58] during sleep.
Sawtooth waves are produced in the transition to and throughout REM sleep. A recent study has reported a large set of regions in the parietal, frontal, and insular cortices that have shown increases in 2–4 Hz power during sawtooth activity, associated with a strong and widespread increase in high frequencies, suggesting that the waves may be involved in cognitive processes during REM sleep [59]. The cortical generators and functional significance of the waves and results on no sex difference in one study included in this review [36] need to be replicated before definite conclusions can be drawn. Because of the association between REM sleep and dreaming state, it is also possible that that the link exists between the intensity of the visual nature of dreaming and the occurrence of sawtooth waves. The validity of these hypothesis requires further study.
PROGRESS-Plus considerations
The EEG data of male and female participants recruited to studies concerning sex difference synthesized in this review likely reflect participants’ diverse and complex brain activity at the time of investigation. This activity reflects, at least partially, aspects of social identity and a variety of socio-cognitive processes on a time continuum uniting sleep and waking states [60]. We aimed to capture participant characteristics and social identities through analysis of PROGRESS-Plus parameters considered in research. We observed that only age and pubertal stage were considered in data analyses of sex difference. The influence of age on sex differences in sleep waves reported in number studies included in this review (Table 1), should be viewed from the lens of social parameters, which received no consideration in data analysis. It is becoming increasingly clear that these parameters have influence on the brain’s structure and function in significant ways that must be considered in sleep research [61, 62].
Study strengths & limitations
This is the first comprehensive systematic review aggregating data from published studies to examine sex differences in different sleep wave parameters. Our subgroup analyses and visual data presentation allows for nuanced comparisons across studies. We have presented data on the PROGRESS-Plus-related characteristics of study samples, allowing discussion of generalizability and external validity of the existing evidence.
We recognise several limitations. To reduce variability and enhance scientific validity, we limited study inclusion to healthy participants; however, among healthy people there are natural hormonal fluctuations, such as menstrual cycle or menopause and testosterone fluctuations [63,64,65], that are expected to introduce variability in sleep parameters. While several studies included in this review noted the use of contraceptives in their female participants [18, 28, 40, 41] and a few shared relevant information about participants’ menstrual cycle [17, 19, 26,27,28, 35], none investigated the role of sex steroid hormones in sleep wave parameters. The absence of this consideration is crucial as it can disrupt sex-specific findings leading to an over-simplification of the study of biological sex differences, which does not allow for discussion concerning sexual dimorphism or lack of thereof [6].
Likewise, confounding influences from other variables, including gender, stress, diet, physical activity, sleep environment, among others [66,67,68,69], may also influence sleep parameters and reported results on sex differences, but such variables were not investigated in the studies included in this review. Another factor to consider is age. While age was considered in the studies, participant age ranges spanned many years, thus requiring us to use broad developmental categories to analyze results without sacrificing significant data points. We emphasise that future studies should investigate sex differences in sleep wave parameters with greater precision in age, as sex-specific physiology is age-dependent. The integration of advanced statistical techniques and machine learning [70, 71] could lead to a more precise identification of sex-specific sleep wave parameters, should they exist, which is hardly feasible through traditional hypothesis-driven analytic methodologies that assume linearity in the relationship between variables.
Additional limitations include heterogeneity and variability in recording methods and definitions of wave band frequencies (Table 2) among the studies included in this review. Small sample sizes and sex distribution imbalances in some studies (Table 1) may have limited the statistical power to detect sex differences.
It is noteworthy that our searchers were limited to studies published in English, leading to a possible exclusion of evidence published in other languages, and thereby affecting the generalizability of results to the global community. To minimize this bias, we searched Embase, which indexes English language publications from Europe and Asia, through which we retrieved relevant publications coming from these regions.
To complete our discussion, we follow with recommendations for future research on the topic of sex differences in sleep wave parameters. The variability in data recording and processing (i.e., manual versus automated), the number of electrodes, and the leads from which the data were recorded may have contributed to variation observed in our evidence synthesis. Even across studies with the same recording protocol (i.e., PSG with EEG or standalone EEG) researchers defined frequency bands differently and performed data manipulations in the analyses of wave parameters. While it is understandable that different approaches to data processing would be required based on the research objective, to report raw/unprocessed data by sex in the future can guide a richer, more nuanced understanding of brain activity in sleep powered by advanced signal processing, machine learning, and deep learning techniques. A consensus between researchers across health pillars [72] on developing best practices to study sex difference in sleep parameters while considering social and environmental factors is of utmost importance to avoid oversimplification of sex effects, promote inclusivity in research, and improve the generalizability of research results.
Conclusions
Sleep research to date has not been successful in producing consistent results on sex differences in sleep wave parameters. We found that high certainty exists in evidence reporting no sex difference in alpha and delta relative power [35, 43]. Among the few findings of moderate certainty reporting sex differences are those that suggest a greater delta slope in female participants [28, 29] and greater normalized delta power in males [5, 35]. All other results were of low or very low certainty. It is possible that the reported results in the studies included in this review reflect unmeasured social parameters, instead of biological sex. Considering that methodological variability may influence findings of studies included in this review, all efforts should be made to standardize and uniform testing procedures across research settings.
Methods
Protocol and registration
We registered the protocol for this systematic review on the prospective register of systematic reviews (PROSPERO) of the Centre for Reviews and Dissemination (Supplementary Material S5). We followed the reporting guideline of the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) to conduct the review (Supplementary Material S6).
Data search and sources
Search strategy
We developed a search strategy (Supplementary Material S7) in collaboration with an information specialist (JB) at a large rehabilitation research-teaching hospital. The search strategy used a mix of keywords and subject headings (e.g. MeSH, Emtree) combined using the Boolean operators AND and OR, and applied the following four concepts: (A) sex differences, (B) EEGs, (C) electrophysiological markers and (D) sleep. Search terms for concept A were sourced from a previous review and used with modifications [73]. When possible, we applied limits to focus on human studies and to exclude conference proceedings. Results were limited to English language publications. No date limits were used.
We searched MEDLINE ALL (Ovid), Embase Classic and Embase (Ovid), APA PsycInfo (Ovid), and Scopus from each database’s inception on November 22, 2021. A supplemental search was conducted in Dissertations and These Global (Proquest) to review relevant dissertations or theses reference lists. We repeated searches on December 11, 2024. We exported results from each database into Endnote and were subsequently imported into the Systematic Review Accelerator’s Deduplicator for duplicate removal before the screening stage. We cross-checked the references list of all included studies.
Eligibility criteria based on the PICOS approach
We defined eligibility criteria for study inclusion a priori, using the PICOS approach:
P (Participants): human participants of all ages who at the time of participation in research were considered healthy, defined as a person who is not treated with medications that cross the blood–brain barrier or diagnosed with any form of illness.
I (Interventions): this element was not applicable as this was a systematic review of observational studies.
C (Comparisons): sex differences in reported outcomes.
O (Outcomes): sleep wave parameters as reported by researchers.
S (Study design): observational studies of any study design (i.e., cohort, cross-sectional, case control, case series) focusing on sex differences in sleep wave parameters, as presented in study objectives and/or hypothesis.
We excluded non-human studies, conference proceedings, case reports, commentaries, reviews, dissertations, and book chapters.
Inclusion and exclusion criteria
We included peer-reviewed studies published in English language that reported on sex differences in sleep wave parameters, including indices derived from these parameters. We excluded studies that focused on a different but concurrent topic in sleep wave (e.g. sleep stage distribution, % sleep stage, and wave frequency, duration, incidence, periodicity, etc.) or had reported sex differences in response to intervention [74,75,76,77,78]. We considered baseline data or data from untreated arm (i.e., control arm) in intervention studies reporting on sex differences in sleep wave parameters in healthy persons. Case reports dissertations and studies with no primary data were excluded.
Data collection and analysis
Selection process
Two review authors (CCKL and TM or TTS and TM) conducted the title/abstract screening. Six review authors (CCKL, RC, SN, FF, TTS, and TM) performed full text review of potentially relevant studies. Studies that did not meet the inclusion criteria were excluded. The senior author reviewed the quality of the first and second levels of screening.
Two review authors (CCKL and RC) edited the data extraction form used by Mollayeva et al. [79] to include PROGRESS-Plus parameters. Five review authors (CCKL, RC, SN, FF, TTS) used the form to extract data from eligible studies. The form included: (i) study identifiers (i.e., author names, publication year, setting, country); (ii) study characterizations (i.e., research objective(s), sample size, inclusion and exclusion criteria using the PROGRESS-Plus lens, etc.); participant characteristics (i.e., mean age and standard deviation, sex, collected/reported PROGRESS-plus characteristics); research study characteristics (i.e., study design, studied wave parameters [frequency, power, density, amplitude, etc.], statistical analysis); and results pertinent to our research objectives. When information was unclear, we contacted study authors to elaborate on the results and provide further details [32, 43].
Six review authors (CCKL, RC, SN, FF, TTS and TM) checked the accuracy of data extraction. We resolved inconsistencies through group discussion.
Synthesis methods
Multiple authors (RC, CCKL, SN, FF, TTS and TM) compiled study characteristics into a table to assess similarities between studies across PICOS criteria, which was then used in further data synthesis by the team. We categorized data by wave frequency or morphology (delta, theta, alpha, sigma, beta, gamma; spindle, sawtooth), investigated parameters (amplitude, connectivity, density, power, etc.), and measured units (e.g., Hz, \(\mu {V}^{2}/s\), \(\mu {V}^{2}\), etc.). Due to variability in age ranges of participants across the studies, we grouped the age-related developmental stages defined by Lally and Valentine-French into fewer categories to facilitate comparison of results [80]. Lally and Valentine-French define the developmental stages as: prenatal (before birth); infancy and toddlerhood (from birth to age 2); early childhood (ages 2–6); middle and late childhood (age 6 to the onset of puberty); adolescence (from the onset of puberty until age 18); emerging adulthood (ages 18–25); early adulthood (ages 25 to 40–45); middle adulthood (ages 40–45 to 60–65); and late adulthood (65 onward) [80]. For our analysis, we combined them into five categories: infancy and toddlerhood (from birth to < age 2), early, middle, and late childhood and adolescence (ages 2 to < 18); emerging and early adulthood (ages 18 to < 45); middle adulthood (ages 45 to < 65); and late adulthood (65 onwards) [80]. We conducted subgroup analyses by frequency wave, sleep wave parameter, and study quality to explore whether results on sex differences varied.
We observed heterogeneity across all PICOS criteria, confirming that the assumptions for conducting a classical meta-analysis were not met [74]. Further, variations in EEG recording techniques, definitions sleep wave parameters, and differences in the study samples and characteristics prevented us from performing data conversions. Therefore, we synthesized data descriptively, using approach proposed by Slavin [75], and denoted differences between the sexes as follows: higher values in males; higher values in females; and for no difference. We used statistical significance of results as reported by study authors. We created figures to summarize the results for each study by age (treated as a continuous variable) and age-related developmental stage to facilitate more refined comparisons and capture any distinct patterns in sex differences across life stages.
Assessment of risk bias in included studies and certainty assessment
We assessed risk of bias in the included studies using the tool developed by the National Institutes of Health (NIH) for observational cohort and cross-sectional studies [76]. The tool addresses the presence of six potential sources of bias within (1) study participation, (2) study attrition (for cohort studies), (3) association bias, (4) outcome measurement bias, (5) confounding and statistical analysis and (6) reporting [78]. The senior author (TM) conducted a training and calibration session on the quality assessment procedure with four review authors (TTS, RC, SN, and FF) on eight included studies, following which the three review authors (TTS, SN, and FF) together with the senior author (TM) applied the tool to each included study, and recorded supporting information for judgements of risk of bias for each domain. In addition to the crude calculation of the number of biases present out of total number of possible, we rated the study quality as follows: (i) excellent (“ + + ”) when all or most of the criteria were fulfilled (i.e. allowing at most one ‘cannot determine’ or ‘not reported’); (ii) good (“ + ”) when half of the criteria were fulfilled; (iii) fair (“−”) when less than half of the criteria were fulfilled (Supplementary Material S4). We did not exclude studies based on the quality assessment, but considered each study’s quality in the data analysis, reporting, and interpretation.
The GRADE system downgrades the evidence from intervention studies by evaluating the extent of study limitations, including risk of bias, inconsistency of associations, imprecision, indirectness, and publication bias. The certainty of evidence in observational studies included in this review was completed qualitatively, following study quality assessment (e.g., excellent, good, and fair) based on the risk of bias assessment across six domains. We assessed the certainty of the evidence as high if two or more excellent quality studies coming from different team of investigators were concordant regarding the observed sex differences in a specific sleep parameter of the same wave band or waveform and no discordant results from studies of equal quality were present. We assessed certainty of evidence as moderate if two or more studies of good and/or excellent quality were concordant in their results, with a maximum of one discordant result in studies of good or excellent quality. We assigned low certainty if at least two fair and/or good quality studies were concordant in results, with a maximum of one discordant result in studies of fair or good quality. In all other situations, we assessed the certainty as very low.
Sensitivity analysis
We conducted sensitivity analyses to examine the robustness of findings. We visually positioned results on the lifespan continuum, to evaluate the consistency of the results among the same age-related developmental stages and reporting EEG parameters, and to see results of each study in reference to all other studies. Finally, we conducted subgroup analyses based on risk of bias assessment and the type and number of PROGRESS-Plus covariates considered in data analyses. This allowed us to evaluate the impact of study quality and participants’ characteristics on the consistency of the results.
Publication bias
Due to the limited number of studies reporting on the same EEG parameter, high heterogeneity in terms of study design, population, and definition of sleep wave parameters and frequency bands (Tables 1, 2), we did not perform evaluation of the publication bias using statistical tests [77]. We applied visual inspection to handle data issues and certainty assessment to provide valuable insights on the state of evidence on the topic [75].
Dealing with missing data
We contacted study authors to verify key study characteristics and obtain missing data. We received responses from two authors [32, 43] on the requested information.
Ethical review
We did not seek ethical approval, as this study did not involve primary data collection.
Availability of data and materials
No datasets were generated or analysed during the current study.
Abbreviations
- APA:
-
American Psychology Association
- CRD:
-
Centre for Reviews and Dissemination
- CNS:
-
Central nervous system
- EEG:
-
Electroencephalography
- Hz:
-
Hertz
- NREM:
-
Non-rapid eye movement
- REM:
-
Rapid eye movement
- NIH:
-
National Institutes of Health
- PROGRESS-Plus:
-
Place of residence, Race/ethnicity/culture/language, Occupation, Gender/ Sex, Religion, Education, Socioeconomic status, Social capital, Other
- PROSPERO:
-
International Prospective Register of Systematic Reviews
- PRISMA:
-
Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses
- MeSH:
-
Medical subject headings
- SD:
-
Standard deviation
- SWS:
-
Slow-wave sleep
- ISO:
-
Infra-slow oscillations
- HFO:
-
High-frequency oscillations
- RCT:
-
Randomized controlled trials
References
Sex and Gender in Health Research – CIHR. https://cihr-irsc.gc.ca/e/50833.html. Accessed 9 Jan 2025.
Peters SAE, Woodward M. A roadmap for sex- and gender-disaggregated health research. BMC Med. 2023. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s12916-023-03060-w.
Jäncke L. Sex/gender differences in cognition, neurophysiology, and neuroanatomy. F1000Res. 2018. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.12688/f1000research.13917.1.
Duffy KA, Epperson CN. Evaluating the evidence for sex differences: a scoping review of human neuroimaging in psychopharmacology research. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2022;47:430–43.
Markovic A, Kaess M, Tarokh L. Gender differences in adolescent sleep neurophysiology: a high-density sleep EEG study. Sci Rep. 2020. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1038/s41598-020-72802-0.
Joel D, Fausto-Sterling A. Beyond sex differences: new approaches for thinking about variation in brain structure and function. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2016. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1098/rstb.2015.0451.
Lagercrantz H, Ringstedt T. Organization of the neuronal circuits in the central nervous system during development. Acta Paediatr. 2001;90:707–15.
Agnati LF, Cortelli P, Pettersson R, Fuxe K. The concept of trophic units in the central nervous system. Prog Neurobiol. 1995;46:561–74.
Manganotti P, Ajčević M, Buoite SA. EEG as a marker of brain plasticity in clinical applications. Handb Clin Neurol. 2022;184:91–104.
Kaongoen N, Choi J, Woo Choi J, Kwon H, Hwang C, Hwang G, et al. The future of wearable EEG: a review of ear-EEG technology and its applications. J Neural Eng. 2023;20:051002.
Short MR, Hernandez-Pavon JC, Jones A, Pons JL. EEG hyperscanning in motor rehabilitation: a position paper. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2021;18:98.
Cave AE, Barry RJ. Sex differences in resting EEG in healthy young adults. Int J Psychophysiol. 2021;161:35–43.
Liao Y, Zhou G, Luo YX. Sex difference in EEG functional connectivity during sleep stages and resting wake state based on weighted phase lag index. Annu Int Conf IEEE Eng Med Biol Soc. 2019;2019:648–51.
Jochmann T, Seibel MS, Jochmann E, Khan S, Hämäläinen MS, Haueisen J. Sex-related patterns in the electroencephalogram and their relevance in machine learning classifiers. Hum Brain Mapp. 2023;44:4848–58.
O’Neill J, Tabish H, Welch V, Petticrew M, Pottie K, Clarke M, et al. Applying an equity lens to interventions: using PROGRESS ensures consideration of socially stratifying factors to illuminate inequities in health. J Clin Epidemiol. 2014;67:56–64.
Karran EL, Cashin AG, Barker T, Boyd MA, Chiarotto A, Dewidar O, et al. Using PROGRESS-plus to identify current approaches to the collection and reporting of equity-relevant data: a scoping review. J Clin Epidemiol. 2023;163:70–8.
Kluge M, Gazea M, Schüssler P, Genzel L, Dresler M, Kleyer S, et al. Ghrelin increases slow wave sleep and stage 2 sleep and decreases stage 1 sleep and REM sleep in elderly men but does not affect sleep in elderly women. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2010;35:297–304.
Armitage R. The distribution of EEG frequencies in REM and NREM sleep stages in healthy young adults. Sleep. 1995;18:334–41.
Armitage R, Hoffmann R, Fitch T, Trivedi M, Rush AJ. Temporal characteristics of delta activity during NREM sleep in depressed outpatients and healthy adults: group and sex effects. Sleep. 2000;23:1–11. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1093/sleep/23.5.1c.
Baker FC, Turlington SR, Colrain I. Developmental changes in the sleep electroencephalogram of adolescent boys and girls. J Sleep Res. 2012;21:59–67.
Yuksel D, Baker FC, Goldstone A, Claudatos SA, Forouzanfar M, Prouty DE, et al. Stress, sleep, and autonomic function in healthy adolescent girls and boys: findings from the NCANDA study. Sleep Health. 2021;7:72–8.
Feinberg I, Higgins LM, Wong YK, Campbell IG. The adolescent decline of NREM delta, an indicator of brain maturation, is linked to age and sex but not to pubertal stage. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. 2006. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1152/ajpregu.00293.2006.
Campbell IG, Grimm KJ, De Bie E, Feinberg I. Sex, puberty, and the timing of sleep EEG measured adolescent brain maturation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2012;109:5740–3.
Campbell IG, Darchia N, Khaw WY, Higgins LM, Feinberg I. Sleep EEG evidence of sex differences in adolescent brain maturation. Sleep. 2005;28:637–43.
Zhang ZY, Campbell IG, Dhayagude P, Espino HC, Feinberg I. Longitudinal analysis of sleep spindle maturation from childhood through late adolescence. J Neurosci. 2021;41:4253–61.
Mongrain V, Carrier J, Dumont M. Chronotype and sex effects on sleep architecture and quantitative sleep EEG in healthy young adults. Sleep. 2005;28:819–27.
Carrier J, Land S, Buysse DJ, Kupfer DJ, Monk TH. The effects of age and gender on sleep EEG power spectral density in the middle years of life (ages 20–60 years old). Psychophysiology. 2001;38:232–42. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1111/1469-8986.3820232.
Carrier J, Viens I, Poirier G, Robillard R, Lafortune M, Vandewalle G, et al. Sleep slow wave changes during the middle years of life. Eur J Neurosci. 2011;33:758–66.
Rosinvil T, Bouvier J, Dubé J, Lafrenière A, Bouchard M, Cyr-Cronier J, et al. Are age and sex effects on sleep slow waves only a matter of electroencephalogram amplitude? Sleep. 2021. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1093/sleep/zsaa186.
Dijk DJ, Beersma DGM, Bloem GM. Sex differences in the sleep EEG of young adults: visual scoring and spectral analysis. Sleep. 1989;12:500–7.
Ma J, Dijk DJ, Svetnik V, Tymofyeyev Y, Ray S, Walsh JK, et al. EEG power spectra response to a 4-h phase advance and gaboxadol treatment in 822 men and women. J Clin Sleep Med. 2011;7:493–501.
Dorokhov VB, Puchkova AN, Shumov DE, Gandina EO, Taranov AO, Ligun NV, et al. The yin and yang of two opponent processes of sleep-wake regulation: Sex-associated differences in the spectral EEG markers of the drives for sleep and wake. Chronobiol Int. 2024;41:1046–57.
Fukuda N, Honma H, Kohsaka M, Kobayashi R, Sakakibara S, Kohsaka S, et al. Gender difference of slow wave sleep in middle aged and elderly subjects. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1999;53:151–3.
Hejazi NS, Duncan WC, Kheirkhah M, Kowalczyk A, Riedner B, Oppenheimer M, et al. Sleep Delta power, age, and sex effects in treatment-resistant depression. J Psychiatr Res. 2024;174:332–9.
Latta F, Leproult R, Tasali E, Hofmann E, Van Cauter E. Sex differences in delta and alpha EEG activities in healthy older adults. Sleep. 2005;28:1525–34.
Luo X, Zhou B, Shi J, Li G, Zhu Y. Effects of gender and age on sleep EEG functional connectivity differences in subjects with mild difficulty falling asleep. Front Psychiatry. 2024. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.3389/fpsyt.2024.1433316.
Mourtazaev MS, Kemp B, Zwinderman AH, Kamphuisen HAC. Age and gender affect different characteristics of slow waves in the sleep EEG. Sleep. 1995;18:557–64.
Pun M, Guadagni V, Longman RS, Hanly PJ, Hill MD, Anderson TJ, et al. Sex differences in the association of sleep spindle density and cognitive performance among community-dwelling middle-aged and older adults with obstructive sleep apnea. J Sleep Res. 2024. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1111/jsr.14095.
Ringli M, Kurth S, Huber R, Jenni OG. The sleep EEG topography in children and adolescents shows sex differences in language areas. Int J Psychophysiol. 2013;89:241–5.
Ujma PP, Dresler M, Simor P, Fabó D, Ulbert I, Erőss L, et al. The sleep EEG envelope is a novel, neuronal firing-based human biomarker. Sci Rep. 2022. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1038/s41598-022-22255-4.
Ujma PP, Konrad BN, Simor P, Gombos F, Körmendi J, Steiger A, et al. Sleep EEG functional connectivity varies with age and sex, but not general intelligence. Neurobiol Aging. 2019;78:87–97.
Ventura S, Mathieson SR, O’Toole JM, Livingstone V, Ryan MA, Boylan GB. Electroencephalographic sleep macrostructure and sleep spindles in early infancy. Sleep. 2022. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1093/sleep/zsab262.
Yoon JE, Oh D, Hwang I, Park JA, Im HJ, Lee SK, et al. Sleep structure and electroencephalographic spectral power of middle-aged or older adults: Normative values by age and sex in the Korean population. J Sleep Res. 2021. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1111/jsr.13358.
EEG Normal Waveforms - StatPearls - NCBI Bookshelf. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK539805/. Accessed 9 Jan 2025.
Watson BO. Cognitive and physiologic impacts of the infraslow oscillation. Front Syst Neurosci. 2018. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.3389/fnsys.2018.00044.
Physiology, Sleep Stages - StatPearls - NCBI Bookshelf. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK526132/. Accessed 9 Jan 2025.
EEG Normal Sleep - StatPearls - NCBI Bookshelf. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK537023/. Accessed 9 Jan 2025.
Mundy-Castle AC. Theta and beta rhythm in the electroencephalograms of normal adults. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1951;3:477–86.
Roca-Stappung M, Fernández T, Becerra J, Mendoza-Montoya O, Espino M, Harmony T. Healthy aging: relationship between quantitative electroencephalogram and cognition. Neurosci Lett. 2012;510:115–20.
Heinbockel H, Quaedflieg CWEM, Schneider TR, Engel AK, Schwabe L. Stress enhances emotional memory-related theta oscillations in the medial temporal lobe. Neurobiol Stress. 2021. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.ynstr.2021.100383.
Ricci A, He F, Calhoun SL, Fang J, Vgontzas AN, Liao D, et al. Sex and pubertal differences in the maturational trajectories of sleep spindles in the transition from childhood to adolescence: a population-based study. eNeuro. 2021. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1523/ENEURO.0257-21.2021.
Nicolas J, King BR, Levesque D, Lazzouni L, Coffey E, Swinnen S, et al. Sigma oscillations protect or reinstate motor memory depending on their temporal coordination with slow waves. Elife. 2022. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.7554/eLife.73930.
Maierova L, Borisuit A, Scartezzini JL, Jaeggi SM, Schmidt C, Münch M. Diurnal variations of hormonal secretion, alertness and cognition in extreme chronotypes under different lighting conditions. Sci Rep. 2016. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1038/srep33591.
Bandarabadi M, Herrera CG, Gent TC, Bassetti C, Schindler K, Adamantidis AR. A role for spindles in the onset of rapid eye movement sleep. Nat Commun. 2020;11:5247.
Schabus M, Dang-Vu TT, Albouy G, Balteau E, Boly M, Carrier J, et al. Hemodynamic cerebral correlates of sleep spindles during human non-rapid eye movement sleep. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007;104:13164–9.
Cowan E, Liu A, Henin S, Kothare S, Devinsky O, Davachi L. Sleep Spindles promote the restructuring of memory representations in ventromedial prefrontal cortex through enhanced hippocampal-cortical functional connectivity. J Neurosci. 2020;40:1909–19.
McCormick DA, Bal T. Sensory gating mechanisms of the thalamus. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1994;4:550–6.
Tomasi D, Chang L, Caparelli EC, Ernst T. Sex differences in sensory gating of the thalamus during auditory interference of visual attention tasks. Neuroscience. 2008;151:1006–15.
Frauscher B, von Ellenrieder N, Dolezalova I, Bouhadoun S, Gotman J, Peter-Derex L. Rapid eye movement sleep sawtooth waves are associated with widespread cortical activations. J Neurosci. 2020;40:8900–12.
Gordon AM, Mendes WB, Prather AA. The social side of sleep: elucidating the links between sleep and social processes. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2017;26:470–5.
Hari R, Henriksson L, Malinen S, Parkkonen L. Centrality of social interaction in human brain function. Neuron. 2015;88:181–93.
Kieckhaefer C, Schilbach L, Bzdok D. Social belonging: brain structure and function is linked to membership in sports teams, religious groups, and social clubs. Cereb Cortex. 2023;33:4405–20.
Empson JAC, Purdie DW. Effects of sex steroids on sleep. Ann Med. 1999;31:141–5.
Andersen ML, Alvarenga TF, Mazaro-Costa R, Hachul HC, Tufik S. The association of testosterone, sleep, and sexual function in men and women. Brain Res. 2011;1416:80–104.
Driver HS, Werth E, Dijk D-J, Borbély AA. The menstrual cycle effects on sleep. Sleep Med Clin. 2008;3:1–11.
Brand S, Gerber M, Beck J, Hatzinger M, Phse U, Holsboer-Trachsler E. Exercising, sleep-EEG patterns, and psychological functioning are related among adolescents. World J Biol Psychiatry. 2010;11:129–40.
Phillips F, Crisp AH, Mcguinness B, Kalucy EC, Chen CN, Koval J, et al. Isocaloric diet changes and electroencephalographic sleep. The Lancet. 1975;306:723–5.
Yap Y, Tung NYC, Collins J, Phillips A, Bei B, Wiley JF. Daily relations between stress and electroencephalography-assessed sleep: a 15-day intensive longitudinal design with ecological momentary assessments. Ann Behav Med. 2022;56:1144–56.
Markovic A, Kaess M, Tarokh L. Environmental factors shape sleep eeg connectivity during early adolescence. Cereb Cortex. 2020;30:5780–91.
Jana S, Sutton M, Mollayeva T, Chan V, Colantonio A, Escobar MD. Application of multiple testing procedures for identifying relevant comorbidities, from a large set, in traumatic brain injury for research applications utilizing big health-administrative data. Front Big Data. 2022. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.3389/fdata.2022.793606.
Pawan DR. Machine learning techniques for electroencephalogram based brain-computer interface: a systematic literature review. Meas Sens. 2023;28:100823.
Canadian Institutes of Health Research. What is health research? 2023. https://www.cihr-irsc.gc.ca/e/53146.html.
Mollayeva T, Mollayeva S, Pacheco N, Colantonio A. Systematic review of sex and gender effects in traumatic brain injury: equity in clinical and functional outcomes. Front Neurol. 2021. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.3389/fneur.2021.678971.
Chapter 10: Analysing data and undertaking meta-analyses | Cochrane Training. https://training.cochrane.org/handbook/current/chapter-10. Accessed 9 Jan 2025.
Slavin RE. Best evidence synthesis: an intelligent alternative to meta-analysis. J Clin Epidemiol. 1995;48:9–18.
Study Quality Assessment Tools | NHLBI, NIH. https://www.nhlbi.nih.gov/health-topics/study-quality-assessment-tools. Accessed 9 Jan 2025.
Lin L, Chu H. Quantifying publication bias in meta-analysis. Biometrics. 2018;74:785–94.
Hayden JA, van der Windt DA, Cartwright JL, Côté P, Bombardier C. Assessing bias in studies of prognostic factors. Ann Intern Med. 2013;158:280–6.
Rizvi ST, Shah JS, Shaaya S, Mollayeva T. Treating cognitive impairments in primary central nervous system infections: a systematic review of pharmacological interventions. Medicine. 2023;102:E34151.
1.4: Periods of Development – Lifespan Development – A Psychological Perspective. https://opentextbooks.concordia.ca/lifespandevelopment/chapter/1-4-periods-of-development/. Accessed 9 Jan 2025.
Funding
This work was supported by the Canada Research Chairs Program for Neurological Disorders and Brain Health (CRC-2021-00074) and in part by the Global Brain Health Institute (GBHI), Alzheimer’s Association, and Alzheimer’s Society UK Pilot Award for Global Brain Health Leaders (GBHI ALZ UK-23-971123). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
RC: Investigation; Formal analysis; Scientific messages; Equity, Diversity, Inclusion considerations; Writing—original draft, review & editing. SN: Investigation; Critical appraisal; Equity, Diversity, Inclusion considerations; Writing – original draft, review & editing; TTS: Data Visualization; Critical appraisal; Equity, Diversity, Inclusion considerations; Writing—review & editing. CCKL: Conceptualization; Methodology; Formal analysis; Investigation; Writing—review & editing; FF: Investigation; Critical appraisal; Equity, Diversity, Inclusion considerations; JB: Conceptualization; Methodology; Searches; Scientific messages; Writing—review & editing; TM: Conceptualization; Methodology; Searches; Investigation; Formal analysis; Critical appraisal; Scientific messages; Equity, Diversity, Inclusion considerations; Resources; Writing—original draft, review & editing; Validation; Funding acquisition; Project Supervision.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Ethics approval and consent to participate
Not applicable.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Chapman, R., Najima, S., Tylinski Sant’Ana, T. et al. Sex differences in electrical activity of the brain during sleep: a systematic review of electroencephalographic findings across the human lifespan. BioMed Eng OnLine 24, 33 (2025). https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s12938-025-01354-z
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s12938-025-01354-z
Keywords
- Electroencephalogram
- EEG
- Equity, diversity, and inclusion
- Method
- Integrated ethics
- Health equity
- PROGRESS-Plus (Place of residence, Race/ethnicity, Occupation, Gender/Sex, Religion, Education, Socioeconomic status, Social capital; other contextual parameters, including age)
- Sex differences
- Social determinants of health
- Inequity
- Neurodiversity
- Neurobiology
- Neuroimaging
- Brain
- Sleep stages